stringtranslate.com

Dominance hierarchy

A high-ranking male mandrill advertises his status with bright facial coloration.[1]

In the zoological field of ethology, a dominance hierarchy (formerly and colloquially called a pecking order) is a type of social hierarchy that arises when members of animal social groups interact, creating a ranking system. A dominant higher-ranking individual is sometimes called an alpha, and a submissive lower-ranking individual is called a beta. Different types of interactions can result in dominance depending on the species, including ritualized displays of aggression or direct physical violence.[2][better source needed] In social living groups, members are likely to compete for access to limited resources and mating opportunities. Rather than fighting each time they meet, individuals of the same sex establish a relative rank, with higher-ranking individuals often gaining more access to resources and mates. Based on repetitive interactions, a social order is created that is subject to change each time a dominant animal is challenged by a subordinate one.

Definitions

Dominance is an individual's preferential access to resources over another based on coercive capacity based on strength, threat, and intimidation, compared to prestige (persuasive capacity based on skills, abilities, and knowledge).[3] A dominant animal is one whose sexual, feeding, aggressive, and other behaviour patterns subsequently occur with relatively little influence from other group members.[4] Subordinate animals are opposite; their behaviour is submissive, and can be relatively easily influenced or inhibited by other group members.[5]

Dominance

Wedge-capped capuchins have a clear dominance hierarchy.

For many animal societies, an individual's position in the dominance hierarchy corresponds with their opportunities to reproduce.[6] In hierarchically social animals, dominant individuals may exert control over others. For example, in a herd of feral goats it is a large male that is dominant and maintains discipline and coherence of the flock. He leads the group but shares leadership on a foraging expedition with a mature she-goat who will normally outlast a succession of dominant males.[7] However, earlier work showed that leadership orders in goats were not related to age or dominance.[8] In sheep, position in a moving flock is highly correlated with social dominance, but there is no definite study to show consistent voluntary leadership by an individual.[9] In birds, dominant individuals preferentially select higher perches to put themselves in the best position to detect and avoid predators, as well as to display their dominance to other members of their own species.[10] It has been suggested that decision-taking about the actions of the group is commonly dissociated from social dominance.[11]

When individuals seek high rank

Given the benefits and costs of possessing a high rank within a hierarchical group, there are certain characteristics of individuals, groups, and environments that determine whether an individual will benefit from a high rank. These include whether or not high rank gives them access to valuable resources such as mates and food. Age, intelligence, experience, and physical fitness can influence whether or not an individual deems it worthwhile to pursue a higher ranking in the hierarchy, which often comes at the expense of conflict. Hierarchy results from interactions, group dynamics, and sharing of resources, so group size and composition affect the dominance decisions of high-ranking individuals. For example, in a large group with many males, it may be difficult for the highest-ranking male to dominate all the mating opportunities, so some mate sharing is likely to exist. These opportunities available to subordinates reduce the likelihood of a challenge to the dominant male: mating is no longer an all-or-nothing game and the sharing is enough to placate most subordinates. Another aspect that can determine dominance hierarchies is the environment. In populations of Kenyan vervet monkeys, high-ranking females have higher foraging success when the food resources are clumped, but when food is distributed throughout an area they lose their advantage, because subordinate females can acquire food with less risk of encountering a dominant female.[12]

Benefits

Foraging success

A benefit to high-ranking individuals is increased foraging success and access to food resources. During times of water shortage the highest-ranking vervet females have greater access than subordinates females to water in tree holes. In chacma baboons, the high-ranking males have the first access to vertebrate prey that has been caught by the group, and in yellow baboons the dominant males feed for longer without being interrupted.[12]

In many bird species, the dominant individuals have higher rates of food intake. Such species include dark-eyed juncos and oystercatchers. The dominant individuals in these groups fill themselves up first and fill up more quickly, so they spend less time foraging, which reduces the risk of predation. Thus they have increased survival because of increased nutrition and decreased predation.[12]

Reproductive success

In primates, a well-studied group, high rank brings reproductive success, as seen in a 1991 meta-analysis of 32 studies.[13] A 2016 study determined that higher status increased reproductive success amongst men, and that this did not vary by type of subsistence (foraging, horticulture, pastoralism, agriculture). This contradicts the "egalitarian hypothesis", which predicts that status would affect reproductive success more amongst foragers than amongst nonforagers.[14]

High-ranking bonnet macaque males have more access to fertile females and consequently partake in most of the matings within the group; in one population, three males were responsible for over 75% of matings. In this population, males often vary in rank. As their rank improves, they gain more exclusive time with fertile females; when their rank decreases, they get less time.[15] In many primates, including bonnet macaques and rhesus monkeys, the offspring of high-ranking individuals have better fitness and thus an increased rate of survival. This is most likely a function of two factors: The first is that high-ranking males mate with high-ranking females. Assuming their high rank is correlated with higher fitness and fighting ability, this trait will be conferred to their offspring. The second factor is that higher-ranking parents probably provide better protection to their offspring and thus ensure higher survival rates.[12] Amongst rhesus macaques, higher-ranking males sired more offspring, though the alpha male was never the one to sire the most offspring, with that instead being a high-ranking but not top male. The complex relationship between rank and reproduction in this species is likely explained by the fact that rhesus macaques queue, rather than fight, for dominance, meaning that the alpha male is not necessarily the strongest or most attractive male.[16][17]

In rodents, the highest-ranking male frequently sires the most offspring. The same pattern is found in most carnivores, such as the dwarf mongoose. The dwarf mongoose lives in a social system with one dominant pair. The dominant female produces all or almost all of the offspring in the living group, and the dominant male has first access to her during her oestrus period. In red deer, the males who experienced winter dominance, resulting from greater access to preferred foraging sites, had higher ability to get and maintain larger harems during the mating season.[12]

In many monogamous bird species, the dominant pairs tend to get the best territories, which in turn promote offspring survival and adult health. In dunnocks, a species of bird that experiences many mating systems, sometimes individuals will form a group that will have one dominant male who achieves all of the mating in the group.[12]

In the monogynous bee species Melipona subnitida, the queen seeks to maintain reproductive success by preventing workers from caring for their cells, pushing or hitting them using her antennae. Workers display aggression towards males, claiming priority over the cells when males try to use them to place eggs.[18][19]

Costs of being dominant

There are costs to being of a high rank in a hierarchical group which offset the benefits. The most common costs to high-ranking individuals are higher metabolic rates and higher levels of stress hormones.[12] In great tits and pied flycatchers, high-ranking individuals experience higher resting metabolic rates and therefore need to consume more food in order to maintain fitness and activity levels than do subordinates in their groups. The energetic costs of defending territory, mates, and other resources can be very consuming and cause high-ranking individuals, who spend more time in these activities, to lose body mass over long periods of dominance. Therefore, their physical condition decreases the longer they spend partaking in these high-energy activities, and they lose rank as a function of age.[12]

In wild male baboons, the highest-ranking male, also known as the alpha, experiences high levels of both testosterone and glucocorticoid, which indicates that high-ranking males undergo higher levels of stress which reduces fitness. Reduced health and longevity occurs because these two hormones have immunosuppressant activity, which reduces survival and presents opportunities for parasitic infestation and other health risks. This reduced fitness due to the alpha position results in individuals maintaining high rank for shorter periods of time and having an overall reduced health and longevity from the physical strain and costs of the position.[20]

Interpersonal complementarity hypothesis

The interpersonal complementarity hypothesis suggests that obedience and authority are reciprocal, complementary processes. That is, it predicts that one group member's behaviours will elicit a predictable set of actions from other group members. Friendly behaviours are predicted to be met with friendly behaviours, and hostile behaviours are predicted to be reciprocated with similar, hostile behaviours. When an individual acts in a dominant, authoritative manner in a group, this behaviour tends to prompt submissive responses from other group members. Similarly, when group members display submissive behaviour, others feel inclined to display dominant behaviours in return. Tiedens and Fragale (2003) found that hierarchical differentiation plays a significant role in liking behaviour in groups. Individuals prefer to interact with other group members whose power, or status behaviour complements their own. That is to say, group members who behave submissively when talking to someone who appears to be in control are better liked, and similarly individuals who display dominant behaviours (e.g., taking charge, issuing orders) are more liked when interacting with docile, subservient individuals.[21]

Subordinance

Benefits

Being subordinate offers a number of benefits. Subordination is beneficial in agonistic conflicts where rank predicts the outcome of a fight. Less injury will occur if subordinate individuals avoid fighting with higher-ranking individuals who would win a large percentage of the time — knowledge of the pecking order keeps both parties from incurring the costs of a prolonged fight. In hens, it has been observed that both dominants and subordinates benefit from a stable hierarchical environment, because fewer challenges means more resources can be dedicated to laying eggs. In groups of highly related individuals, kin selection may influence the stability of hierarchical dominance. A subordinate individual closely related to the dominant individual may benefit more genetically by assisting the dominant individual to pass on their genes.[22]

Alpha male savanna baboons have high levels of testosterone and stress; over a long period of time, this can lead to decreased fitness. The lowest-ranking males also had high stress levels, suggesting that it is the beta males that gain the most fitness, avoiding stress but receiving some of the benefits of moderate rank.[20] The mating tactics of savanna baboons are correlated with their age. Older, subordinate males form alliances to combat higher-ranking males and get access to females.[23]

Fighting with dominant males is a risky behavior that may result in defeat, injury or even death. In bighorn sheep, however, subordinates occasionally win a fight for a female, and they father 44% of the lambs born in the population. These sheep live in large flocks, and dominance hierarchies are often restructured each breeding season.[24]

Burying beetles, which have a social order involving one dominant male controlling most access to mates, display a behavior known as sneak copulation. While one male at a carcass has a 5:1 mating advantage, subordinate males will tempt females away from the carcass with pheromones and attempt to copulate before the dominant male can drive them forcefully away.[25] In flat lizards, young males take advantage of their underdeveloped secondary sex characteristics to engage in sneak copulations. These young males mimic all the visual signs of a female lizard in order to successfully approach a female and copulate without detection by the dominant male. This strategy does not work at close range because the chemical signals given off by the sneaky males reveal their true nature, and they are chased out by the dominant.[26]

Costs to subordinates

Subordinate individuals suffer a range of costs from dominance hierarchies, one of the most notable being reduced access to food sources. When a resource is obtained, dominant individuals are first to feed as well as taking the longest time. Subordinates also lose out in shelter and nesting sites. Brown hyenas, which display defined linear dominance in both sexes, allow subordinate males and females decreased time of feeding at a carcass.[27] In toque monkeys subordinates are often displaced from feeding sites by dominant males. Additionally, they are excluded from sleeping sites, and they suffer reduced growth and increased mortality.[28]

Subordinate individuals often demonstrate a huge reproductive disadvantage in dominance hierarchies. Among brown hyenas, subordinate females have less opportunity to rear young in the communal den, and thus have fewer surviving offspring than do high-ranking individuals. Subordinate males copulate far less with females than do the high-ranking males.[27] In African wild dogs which live in social packs separated into male and female hierarchies, top-ranking alpha females have been observed to produce 76–81% of all litters.[29]

Mitigating the costs

Subordinate animals engage in a number of behaviors in order to outweigh the costs of low rank. Dispersal is often associated with increased mortality and subordination may decrease the potential benefits of leaving the group. In the red fox it has been shown that subordinate individuals, given the opportunity to desert, often do not due to the risk of death and the low possibility that they would establish themselves as dominant members in a new group.[30]

Conflict over dominance

Animal decisions regarding involvement in conflict are defined by the interplay between the costs and benefits of agonistic behaviors. When initially developed, game theory, the study of optimal strategies during pair-wise conflict, was grounded in the false assumption that animals engaged in conflict were of equal fighting ability. Modifications, however, have provided increased focus on the differences between the fighting capabilities of animals and raised questions about their evolutionary development. These differences are believed to determine the outcomes of fights, their intensity, and animal decisions to submit or continue fighting. The influence of aggression, threats, and fighting on the strategies of individuals engaged in conflict has proven integral to establishing social hierarchies reflective of dominant-subordinate interactions.[31]

The asymmetries between individuals have been categorized into three types of interactions:[32]

  1. Resource-holding potential: Animals that are better able to defend resources often win without much physical contact.[32]
  2. Resource value: Animals more invested in a resource are likely to invest more in the fight despite potential for incurring higher costs.[32]
  3. Intruder retreats: When participants are of equal fighting ability and competing for a certain territory, the resident of the territory is likely to end as the victor because he values the territory more. This can be explained further by looking at the example of the common shrews. If one participant believes he is the resident of the territory, he will win when the opponent is weaker or food is scarce. However, if both shrews believe they are the true territory holder, the one with the greater need for food, and therefore, the one that values the resource more, is most likely to win.[32]

As expected, the individual who emerges triumphant is rewarded with the dominant status, having demonstrated their physical superiority. However, the costs incurred to the defeated, which include loss of reproductive opportunities and quality food, can hinder the individual's fitness. In order to minimize these losses, animals generally retreat from fighting or displaying fighting ability unless there are obvious cues indicating victory. These often involve characteristics that provide an advantage during agonistic behavior, such as size of body, displays, etc. Red stags, for example, engage in exhausting roaring contests to exhibit their strength.[32] However, such an activity would impose more costs than benefits for unfit stags, and compel them to retreat from the contest. Larger stags have also been known to make lower-frequency threat signals, acting as indicators of body size, strength, and dominance.[32]

Engaging in agonistic behavior can be very costly and thus there are many examples in nature of animals who achieve dominance in more passive ways. In some, the dominance status of an individual is clearly visible, eliminating the need for agonistic behavior. In wintering bird flocks, white-crowned sparrows display a unique white plumage; the higher the percentage of the crown that consists of white feathers, the higher the status of the individual.[33] For other animals, the time spent in the group serves as a determinant of dominance status. Rank may also be acquired from maternal dominance rank. In rhesus monkeys, offspring gain dominance status based on the rank of the mother—the higher ranked the mother, the higher ranked the offspring will be (Yahner). Similarly, the status of a male Canada goose is determined by the rank of his family. Although dominance is determined differently in each case, it is influenced by the relationships between members of social groups.[34]

Regulation mechanisms

Individuals with greater hierarchical status tend to displace those ranked lower from access to space, to food and to mating opportunities. Thus, individuals with higher social status tend to have greater reproductive success by mating more often and having more resources to invest in the survival of offspring. Hence, hierarchy serves as an intrinsic factor for population control, ensuring adequate resources for the dominant individuals and thus preventing widespread starvation. Territorial behavior enhances this effect.[35]

In eusocial animals

The suppression of reproduction by dominant individuals is the most common mechanism that maintains the hierarchy. In eusocial mammals this is mainly achieved by aggressive interactions between the potential reproductive females. In eusocial insects, aggressive interactions are common determinants of reproductive status, such as in the bumblebee Bombus bifarius,[36] the paper wasp Polistes annularis[37] and in the ants Dinoponera australis and D. quadriceps.[38] In general, aggressive interactions are ritualistic and involve antennation (drumming), abdomen curling and very rarely mandible bouts and stinging. The winner of the interaction may walk over the subordinated, that in turn assumes a prostrated posture. To be effective, these regulatory mechanisms must include traits that make an individual rank position readily recognizable by its nestmates. The composition of the lipid layer on the cuticle of social insects is the clue used by nestmates to recognize each other in the colony, and to discover each insect's reproductive status (and rank).[39] Visual cues may also transmit the same information. Paper wasps Polistes dominulus have individual "facial badges" that permit them to recognize each other and to identify the status of each individual. Individuals whose badges were modified by painting were aggressively treated by their nestmates; this makes advertising a false ranking status costly, and may help to suppress such advertising.[40]

Other behaviors are involved in maintaining reproductive status in social insects. The removal of a thoracic sclerite in Diacamma ants inhibits ovary development; the only reproductive individual of this naturally queenless genus is the one that retains its sclerite intact. This individual is called a gamergate, and is responsible for mutilating all the newly emerged females, to maintain its social status. Gamergates of Harpegnathos saltator arise from aggressive interactions, forming a hierarchy of potential reproductives.[41]

In the honey bee Apis mellifera, a pheromone produced by the queen mandibular glands is responsible for inhibiting ovary development in the worker caste.[42] "Worker policing" is an additional mechanism that prevents reproduction by workers, found in bees and ants. Policing may involve oophagy and immobilization of workers who lay eggs.[43] In some ant species such as the carpenter ant Camponotus floridanus, eggs from queens have a peculiar chemical profile that workers can distinguish from worker laid eggs. When worker-laid eggs are found, they are eaten.[44] In some species, such as Pachycondyla obscuricornis, workers may try to escape policing by shuffling their eggs within the egg pile laid by the queen.[45]

Hormonal control

Modulation of hormone levels after hibernation may be associated with dominance hierarchies in the social order of the paper wasp (Polistes dominulus).[46] This depends on the queen (or foundress), possibly involving specific hormones. Laboratory experiments have shown that when foundresses are injected with juvenile hormone, responsible for regulating growth and development in insects including wasps, the foundresses exhibit an increase in dominance.[46] Further, foundresses with larger corpora allata, a region of the female wasp brain responsible for the synthesis and secretion of juvenile hormone, are naturally more dominant.[46]A follow-up experiment utilized 20-hydroxyecdysone, an ecdysone known to enhance maturation and size of oocytes.[46] The size of the oocytes plays a significant role in establishing dominance in the paper wasp.[47] Foundresses treated with 20-hydroxyecdysone showed more dominance than did those treated with juvenile hormone, so 20-hydroxyecdysone may play a larger role in establishing dominance (Roseler et al., 1984). Subsequent research however, suggests that juvenile hormone is implicated, though only on certain individuals. When injected with juvenile hormone, larger foundresses showed more mounting behaviors than smaller ones, and more oocytes in their ovaries.[47]

The effect of relative rank on stress hormone levels in savanna baboons[20]

Naked mole-rats (Heterocephalus glaber) similarly have a dominance hierarchy dependent on the highest ranking female (queen) and her ability to suppress critically important reproductive hormones in male and female sub-dominants. In sub-dominant males, it appears that luteinizing hormone and testosterone are suppressed, while in females it appears that the suppression involves the entire suppression of the ovarian cycle. This suppression reduces sexual virility and behavior and thus redirects the sub-dominant's behavior into helping the queen with her offspring,[48] though the mechanisms of how this is accomplished are debated. Former research suggests that primer pheromones secreted by the queen cause direct suppression of these vital reproductive hormones and functions however current evidence suggests that it is not the secretion of pheromones which act to suppress reproductive function but rather the queen's extremely high levels of circulating testosterone, which cause her to exert intense dominance and aggressiveness on the colony and thus "scare" the other mole-rats into submission.[49] Research has shown that removal of the queen from the colony allows the reestablishment of reproductive function in sub-dominant individuals. To see if a priming pheromone secreted by the queen was indeed causing reproductive suppression, researchers removed the queen from the colony but did not remove her bedding. They reasoned that if a primer pheromones were on the bedding then the sub-dominant's reproductive function should continue to be suppressed. Instead however, they found that the sub-dominants quickly regained reproductive function even in the presence of the queen's bedding and thus it was concluded that primer pheromones do not seem to play a role in suppressing reproductive function.[49]

Glucocorticoids, signaling molecules which stimulate the fight or flight response, may be implicated in dominance hierarchies. Higher ranking individuals tend to have much higher levels of circulating glucocorticoids than subdominant individuals,[50] the opposite of what had been expected.[51] Two core hypotheses attempt to explain this. The first suggests that higher ranking individuals exert more energy and thus need higher levels of glucocorticoids to mobilize glycogen for energy use.[52] This is supported by the fact that when food availability is low, cortisol levels increase within the dominant male.[51] The second suggests that elevated stress hormones are a result of social factors, particularly when the hierarchy is in transition, perhaps resulting in increased aggression and confrontation. As a result, the dominant individual fights more and has elevated glucocorticoids during this period. Field studies of olive baboons in Kenya seem to support this, as dominant individuals had lower cortisol levels in a stable hierarchy than did subdominant individuals, but the reverse was true at unstable times.[53][51]

Brain pathways and hierarchy

Several areas of the brain contribute to hierarchical behavior in animals. One of the areas that has been linked with this behavior is the prefrontal cortex, a region involved with decision making and social behavior. High social rank in a hierarchical group of mice has been associated with increased excitability in the medial prefrontal cortex of pyramidal neurons, the primary excitatory cell type of the brain.[54] High ranking macaques have a larger rostral prefrontal cortex in large social groups.[55] Neuroimaging studies with computer stimulated hierarchal conditions showed increased activity in the ventral and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, one processing judgment cues and the other processing status of an individual. Other studies have determined that lesions to the prefrontal cortex (when the area is severed to disrupt functioning to observe its role in behavior) led to deficits in processing social hierarchy cues, suggesting this area is important in regulating this information.[56] Although the prefrontal cortex has been implicated, there are other downstream targets of the prefrontal cortex that have also been linked in maintaining this behavior. This includes the amygdala through lesion studies in rats and primates which led to disruption in hierarchy, and can affect the individual negatively or positively depending on the subnuclei that is targeted. Additionally, the dorsal medial PFC-medial dorsal thalamus connection has been linked with maintenance of rank in mice.[57] Another area that has been associated is the dorsal raphe nucleus, the primary serotonergic nuclei (a neurotransmitter involved with many behaviors including reward and learning). In manipulation studies of this region, there were changes in fighting and affiliative behavior in primates and crustaceans.[56]

In specific groups

Female dominance in mammals

The bonobo is one of the few mammals with female-biased dominance.

Female-biased dominance occurs rarely in mammals. It occurs when all adult males exhibit submissive behavior to adult females in social settings. These social settings are usually related to feeding, grooming, and sleeping site priority. It is observed consistently in hyenas, lemurs and the bonobo.[58] The ring-tailed lemur is observed to be the most prominent model of female dominance.[59]

There are three basic proposals for the evolution of female dominance:[60]

  1. The Energy Conservation Hypothesis: males subordinate to females to conserve energy for intense male-male competition experienced during very short breeding seasons
  2. Female behavioral strategy: dominance helps females deal with the unusually high reproductive demands; they prevail in more social conflicts because they have more at stake in terms of fitness.
  3. Male behavioral strategy: males defer as a parental investment because it ensures more resources in a harsh unpredictable climate for the female, and thus, the male's future offspring.

In lemurs, no single hypothesis fully explains female social dominance at this time and all three are likely to play a role. Adult female lemurs have increased concentrations of androgens when they transition from non-breeding to breeding seasons, increasing female aggression.[61][62] Androgens are greater in pregnant female lemurs, which suggests that organizational androgens might influence the developing offspring.[63] Organizational androgens play a role in "explaining female social dominance" in ring-tailed lemurs, as androgens are associated with aggressive behavior in young females.[64] Females that were "exposed to greater concentrations of maternal [androstenedione] late in fetal development were less likely to be aggressed against postnatally, whereas females that were...exposed to greater concentrations of maternal [testosterone]...were more likely to receive aggression postnatally".[64] Dominance rank in female chimpanzees is correlated with reproductive success. Although a high rank is an advantage for females, clear linear hierarchies in female chimpanzees have not been detected.[65] In "masculinized" female mammals like the spotted hyena (Crocuta crocuta), androgens (i.e. specifically, androstenedione and testosterone) are "implicated in the organization and activation of...nonreproductive behavioral traits, including aggression, social dominance, rough-and-tumble play, and scent marking".[66] For aggressively dominant female meerkats (Suricata suricatta), they have "exceptionally high concentrations" of androgens, "particularly during gestation".[67]

Birds

Bottom-rank chicken showing feather damage from pecking by other hens

The concept of dominance, originally called "pecking order", was described in birds by Thorleif Schjelderup-Ebbe in 1921 under the German terms Hackordnung or Hackliste and introduced into English in 1927.[68] In his 1924 German-language article, he noted that "defense and aggression in the hen is accomplished with the beak".[69] This emphasis on pecking led many subsequent studies on fowl behaviour to use it as a primary observation; however, it has been noted that roosters tend to leap and use their claws in conflicts.[70]

Wild and feral chickens form relatively small groups, usually including no more than 10 to 20 individuals. It has been shown that in larger groups, which is common in farming, the dominance hierarchy becomes less stable and aggression increases.[71]

Dominance hierarchies are found in many species of bird. For example, the blue-footed booby brood of two chicks always has a dominance hierarchy due to the asynchronous hatching of the eggs. One egg is laid four days before the other, and incubation starts immediately after laying, so the elder chick is hatched four days before the younger chick and has a four-day head start on growth. The elder, stronger chick almost always becomes the dominant chick. During times of food shortage, the dominant chick often kills the subordinate chick by either repeatedly pecking or by ousting the younger chick from the nest. The brood hierarchy makes it easier for the subordinate chick to die quietly in times of food scarcity, which provides an efficient system for booby parents to maximize their investment.[72]

Eusocial insects

In insect societies, only one to few individuals members of a colony can reproduce, whereas the other colony members have their reproductive capabilities suppressed. This conflict over reproduction in some cases results in a dominance hierarchy. Dominant individuals in this case are known as queens and have the obvious advantage of performing reproduction and benefiting from all the tasks performed by their subordinates, the worker caste (foraging, nest maintenance, nest defense, brood care and thermal regulation). According to Hamilton's rule, the reproduction costs of the worker caste are compensated by the contribution of workers to the queen's reproductive success, with which they share genes. This is true not only for the popular social insects (ants, termites, some bees and wasps), but also for the naked mole-rat Heterocephalus glaber. In a laboratory experiment, Clarke and Faulkes (1997) demonstrated that reproductive status in a colony of H. glaber was correlated with the individual's ranking position within a dominance hierarchy, but aggression between potential reproductives only started after the queen was removed.[73]

The social insects mentioned above, excluding termites, are haplodiploid. Queen and workers are diploid, but males develop from haploid genotypes. In some species, suppression of ovary development is not totally achieved in the worker caste, which opens the possibility of reproduction by workers. Since nuptial flights are seasonal and workers are wingless, workers are almost always non-breeders, and (as gamergate ants or laying worker bees) can only lay unfertilised eggs. These eggs are in general viable, developing into males. A worker that performs reproduction is considered a "cheater" within the colony, because it leaves disproportionately more descendants than do its sisters and mother. The advantage of remaining functionally sterile is only accomplished if every worker assume this "compromise". When one or more workers start reproducing, the "social contract" is destroyed and the colony cohesion is dissolved. Aggressive behavior derived from this conflict may result in the formation of hierarchies, and attempts of reproduction by workers are actively suppressed. In some wasps, such as Polistes fuscatus, instead of not laying eggs, the female workers begin being able to reproduce, but once being under the presence of dominant females, the subordinate female workers can no longer reproduce.[74]

In some wasp species such as Liostenogaster flavolineata there are many possible queens that inhabit a nest, but only one can be queen at a time. When a queen dies the next queen is selected by an age-based dominance hierarchy. This is also true in the species Polistes instabilis, where the next queen is selected based on age rather than size. Polistes exclamans also exhibits this type of hierarchy.[75] Within the dominance hierarchies of the Polistes versicolor, however, the dominant-subordinate context in the yellow paper wasps is directly related to the exchange of food. Future foundresses within the nest compete over the shared resources of nourishment, such as protein. Unequal nourishment is often what leads to the size differences that result in dominant-subordinate position rankings. Therefore, if during the winter aggregate, the female is able to obtain greater access to food, the female could thus reach a dominant position.[76]

In some species, especially in ants, more than one queen can be found in the same colony, a condition called polygyny. In this case, another advantage of maintaining a hierarchy is to prolong the colony lifespan. The top ranked individuals may die or lose fertility and "extra queens" may benefit from starting a colony in the same site or nest. This advantage is critical in some ecological contexts, such as in situations where nesting sites are limited or dispersal of individuals is risky due to high rates of predation. This polygynous behavior has also been observed in some eusocial bees such as Schwarziana quadripunctata. In this species, multiple queens of varying sizes are present. The larger, physogastric, queens typically control the nest, though a "dwarf" queen will take its place in the case of a premature death.[77]

Variations

Spectrum of social systems

Dominance hierarchies emerge as a result of intersexual and intrasexual selection within groups, where competition between individuals results in differential access to resources and mating opportunities. This can be mapped across a spectrum of social organization ranging from egalitarian to despotic, varying across multiple dimensions of cooperation and competition in between.[78] Conflict can be resolved in multiple ways, including aggression, tolerance, and avoidance. These are produced by social decision-making, described in the "relational model" created by the zoologist Frans De Waal.[79] In systems where competition between and within the sexes is low, social behaviour gravitates towards tolerance and egalitarianism, such as that found in woolley spider monkeys.[80][81] In despotic systems where competition is high, one or two members are dominant while all other members of the living group are equally submissive, as seen in Japanese and rhesus macaques, leopard geckos, dwarf hamsters, gorillas, the cichlid Neolamprologus pulcher, and African wild dog.[82][83] Linear ranking systems, or "pecking orders", which tend to fall in between egalitarianism and despotism, follow a structure where every member of the group is recognized as either dominant or submissive relative to every other member. This results in a linear distribution of rank, as seen in spotted hyenas and brown hyenas.[84]

Context dependency

Eringer cattle competing for dominance

Dominance and its organisation can be highly variable depending on the context or individuals involved. In European badgers, dominance relationships may vary with time as individuals age, gain or lose social status, or change their reproductive condition.[85] Dominance may also vary across space in territorial animals as territory owners are often dominant over all others in their own territory but submissive elsewhere, or dependent on the resource. Even with these factors held constant, perfect dominance hierarchies are rarely found in groups of any great size, at least in the wild.[11] Dominance hierarchies in small herds of domestic horses are generally linear hierarchies whereas in large herds the relationships are triangular.[86] Dominance hierarchies can be formed at a very early age. Domestic piglets are highly precocious and, within minutes of being born, or sometimes seconds, will attempt to suckle. The piglets are born with sharp teeth and fight to develop a teat order as the anterior teats produce a greater quantity of milk. Once established, this teat order remains stable with each piglet tending to feed from a particular teat or group of teats.[87] Dominance–subordination relationships can vary markedly between breeds of the same species. Studies on Merinos and Border Leicesters sheep revealed an almost linear hierarchy in the Merinos but a less rigid structure in the Border Leicesters when a competitive feeding situation was created.[9]

Species with egalitarian/non-linear hierarchies

Although many group-living animal species have a hierarchy of some form, some species have more fluid and flexible social groupings, where rank does not need to be rigidly enforced, and low-ranking group members may enjoy a wider degree of social flexibility. Some animal societies are "democratic", with low-ranking group members being able to influence which group member is leader and which one is not. Sometimes dominant animals must maintain alliances with subordinates and grant them favours to receive their support in order to retain their dominant rank. In chimpanzees, the alpha male may need to tolerate lower-ranking group members hovering near fertile females[88] or taking portions of his meals.[89] Other examples can include Muriqui monkeys. Within their groups, there is abundant food and females will mate promiscuously. Because of this, males gain very little in fighting over females, who are, in turn, too large and strong for males to monopolize or control, so males do not appear to form especially prominent ranks between them, with several males mating with the same female in view of each other.[90] This type of mating style is also present in manatees, removing their need to engage in serious fighting.[91] Among female elephants, leadership roles are not acquired by sheer brute force, but instead through seniority, and other females can collectively show preferences for where the herd can travel.[92] In hamadryas baboons, several high-ranking males will share a similar rank, with no single male being an absolute leader.[93] Female bats also have a somewhat fluid social structure, in which rank is not strongly enforced.[94] Bonobos are matriarchal, yet their social groups are also generally quite flexible, and serious aggression is quite rare between them.[95] In olive baboons, certain animals are dominant in certain contexts, but not in others. Prime age male olive baboons claim feeding priority, yet baboons of any age or sex can initiate and govern the group's collective movements.

See also

References

  1. ^ Leigh, Steven R.; Setchell, Joanna M.; Charpentier, Marie; et al. (2008). "Canine tooth size and fitness in male mandrills (Mandrillus sphinx)". Journal of Human Evolution. 55 (1): 75–85. doi:10.1016/j.jhevol.2008.01.001. PMID 18472142.
  2. ^ Yeh J (11 June 2018). "Dominance Hierarchy". Encyclopedia.com.
  3. ^ Cheng, Joey T. (2020). "Dominance, prestige, and the role of leveling in human social hierarchy and equality". Current Opinion in Psychology (Review). 33: 238–244. doi:10.1016/j.copsyc.2019.10.004. PMID 31794955. S2CID 208627517. Considerable evidence now indicates that in humans social stratification is principally based jointly on dominance (coercive capacity based on strength, threat, and intimidation) and prestige (persuasive capacity based on skills, abilities, and knowledge). Although intimidation can beget compliance, hierarchical relationships based on dominance are relatively less stable.
  4. ^ Drews, Carlos (1993). "The concept and definition of dominance in animal behaviour". Behaviour. 125 (3–4): 283–313. doi:10.1163/156853993X00290.
  5. ^ Burgoon, J.; Johnson, M.; Koch, P. (1998). "The nature and measurement of interpersonal dominance". Communication Monographs. 65 (4): 308–335. doi:10.1080/03637759809376456.
  6. ^ Cant, Michael A.; Field, Jeremy (2005). "Helping effort in a dominance hierarchy". Behavioral Ecology. 16 (4): 708–715. doi:10.1093/beheco/ari051.
  7. ^ MacKenzie, D. (1980). Goat Husbandry (4th, revised and edited by Jean Laing ed.). London and Boston: Faber and Faber. pp. 66–85.
  8. ^ Stewart, J. C.; Scott, J. P. (1947). "Lack of correlation between leadership and dominance relationships in a herd of goats". Journal of Comparative Physiological Psychology. 40 (4): 255–264. doi:10.1037/h0060710. PMID 20260236.
  9. ^ a b Squires, V. R.; Daws, G. T. (1975). "Leadership and dominance relationships in Merino and Border Leicester sheep". Applied Animal Ethology. 1 (3): 263–274. doi:10.1016/0304-3762(75)90019-X.
  10. ^ Portugal, S. J.; Sivess, L.; Martin, G. R.; et al. (2017). "Perch height predicts dominance rank in birds". Ibis. 159 (2): 456–462. doi:10.1111/ibi.12447.
  11. ^ a b Rowell, T. E. (1974). "The concept of social dominance". Behavioral Biology. 11 (2): 131–154. doi:10.1016/S0091-6773(74)90289-2. PMID 4367951.
  12. ^ a b c d e f g h Huntingford, Felicity; Turner, Angela K. (1987). Animal Conflict. London: Chapman and Hall. pp. 39–54.
  13. ^ Cowlishaw, Guy; Dunbar, Robin I. M. (1991). "Dominance rank and mating success in male primates". Animal Behaviour. 41 (6): 1045–1056. doi:10.1016/s0003-3472(05)80642-6. S2CID 53190498.
  14. ^ Von Rueden, Christopher R.; Jaeggi, Adrian V. (2016). "Men's status and reproductive success in 33 nonindustrial societies: Effects of subsistence, marriage system, and reproductive strategy". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 113 (39): 10824–10829. Bibcode:2016PNAS..11310824V. doi:10.1073/pnas.1606800113. PMC 5047206. PMID 27601650.
  15. ^ Samuels, A.; Silk, J. B.; Rodman, P. (1984). "Changes in the dominance rank and reproductive behavior of male bonnet macaques (Macaca radiate)". Anim. Behav. 32 (4): 994–1003. doi:10.1016/s0003-3472(84)80212-2. S2CID 53186523.
  16. ^ Widdig, Anja; Kessler, Matthew J.; Bercovitch, Fred B.; et al. (2016). "Genetic studies on the Cayo Santiago rhesus macaques: a review of 40 years of research". American Journal of Primatology. 78 (1): 44–62. doi:10.1002/ajp.22424. PMID 26031601. S2CID 32784846.
  17. ^ Dubuc, Constance; Muniz, Laura; Heistermann, Michael; Engelhardt, Antje; Widdig, Anja (2011). "Testing the priority-of-access model in a seasonally breeding primate species". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 65 (8): 1615–1627. doi:10.1007/s00265-011-1172-8. PMC 3134767. PMID 21874084.
  18. ^ Bonnatti, Vanessa; Luz Paulino Simões, Zilá; Franco, Fernando Faria; Tiago, Mauricio (3 January 2014). "Evidence of at least two evolutionary lineages in Melipona subnitida (Apidae, Meliponini) suggested by mtDNA variability and geometric morphometrics of forewings". Naturwissenschaften. 101 (1): 17–24. Bibcode:2014NW....101...17B. doi:10.1007/s00114-013-1123-5. PMID 24384774. S2CID 18986069.
  19. ^ Koedam, D.; Contrera, A. de O. Fidalgo; Imperatriz-Fonseca, V. L. (2004). "How queen and workers share in male production in the stingless bee Melipona subnitida Ducke (Apidae, Meliponini)". Insectes Sociaux. 52 (2): 114–121. doi:10.1007/s00040-004-0781-x. S2CID 46413244.
  20. ^ a b c Gesquiere, Laurence R.; Learn, Niki H.; Simao, Carolina M.; et al. (2011). "Life at the Top: Rank and Stress in Wild Male Baboons". Science. 333 (6040): 357–60. Bibcode:2011Sci...333..357G. doi:10.1126/science.1207120. PMC 3433837. PMID 21764751.
  21. ^ Tiedens, Larissa; Fragale, Alison (2003). "Power moves: Complementarity in dominant and submissive nonverbal behavior". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 84 (3): 558–568. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.319.709. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.84.3.558. PMID 12635916.
  22. ^ Pusey, A. E.; Packer, C. (1997). "The ecology of relationships". In J. R. Krebs; N. B. Davies (eds.). Behavioural Ecology: An Evolutionary Approach. Oxford: Blackwell Science. pp. 254–283.
  23. ^ Noe, R.; Sluijter, A. A. (1990). "Reproductive Tactics of Male Savanna Baboons". Behaviour. 113 (1–2): 117–170. doi:10.1163/156853990x00455. S2CID 14258881.
  24. ^ Hogg, J. T.; Forbes, S. H. (1997). "Mating in bighorn sheep: Frequent male reproduction via a high-risk unconventional tactic". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 41 (1): 33–48. doi:10.1007/s002650050361. S2CID 8265935.
  25. ^ Pettinger, Adam M.; Steiger, Sandra; Mueller, Josef K.; et al. (2011). "Dominance status and carcass availability affect the outcome of sperm competition in burying beetles". Behavioral Ecology. 22 (5): 1079–1087. doi:10.1093/beheco/arr093.
  26. ^ Whiting, Martin J.; Webb, Jonathan K.; Keogh, J. Scott (2009). "Flat lizard female mimics use sexual deception in visual but not chemical signals". Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 276 (1662): 1585–1591. doi:10.1098/rspb.2008.1822. PMC 2660994. PMID 19324828.
  27. ^ a b Owens, D.; Owens, M. (1996). "Social dominance and reproductive patterns in brown hyaenas, Hyaena brunnea, of the central Kalahari desert". Animal Behaviour. 51 (3): 535–551. doi:10.1006/anbe.1996.0058. S2CID 53163212.
  28. ^ Dittus, W. P. J. (1977). "The Social Regulation of Population Density and Age-Sex Distribution in the Toque Monkey". Behaviour. 63 (3): 281–322. doi:10.1163/156853977x00450.
  29. ^ Creel, S. (1997). "Handling of African wild dogs and chronic stress: Reply". Conservation Biology. 11 (6): 1454–1456. doi:10.1046/j.1523-1739.1997.0110061454.x. S2CID 85088576.
  30. ^ Baker, P. J.; Robertson, C. P. J.; Funk, S. M.; Harris, S. (1998). "Potential fitness benefits of group living in the red fox, Vulpes vulpes". Animal Behaviour. 56 (6): 1411–1424. doi:10.1006/anbe.1998.0950. PMID 9933538. S2CID 24702890.
  31. ^ Chase, I. D.; Tovey, C.; Murch, P. (2003). "Two's Company, Three's a Crowd: Differences in Dominance Relationships in Isolated versus Socially Embedded Pairs of Fish". Behaviour. 140 (10): 1193–217. doi:10.1163/156853903771980558. S2CID 56345496.
  32. ^ a b c d e f Huntingford, Felicity; Turner, Angela K. (1987). Animal Conflict. London: Chapman and Hall. pp. 156–159, 194–205, 208–209, 250–253.
  33. ^ Laubach, Zachary (27 November 2012). Functional Importance of Plumage Badges as Intraspecific Signals in White-Crowned Sparrows (zonotrichia Leucophrys Oriantha) (Thesis). Deep Blue at the University of Michigan. hdl:2027.42/77948.
  34. ^ Yahner, Richard H. (2012). Wildlife Behavior and Conservation. New York: Springer. pp. 95–100.
  35. ^ "Behavior: The Animal Watchers". Time. 22 October 1973. Archived from the original on 14 December 2008. Retrieved 2010-05-01.
  36. ^ Foster, R. L.; Ameilia, B.; Verdirame, D.; O'Donnell, S. (2004). "Reproductive physiology, dominance interactions, and division of labour among bumble bee workers". Physiological Entomology. 29 (4): 327–334. doi:10.1111/j.0307-6962.2004.00388.x. S2CID 34826563.
  37. ^ Hughes, C. R.; Beck, M. O.; Strassman, J. E. (1987). "Queen succession in the social wasp Polistes annularis". Ethology. 76 (2): 124–132. Bibcode:1987Ethol..76..124H. doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310.1987.tb00678.x.
  38. ^ Monnin, T.; Ratnieks, F. L. W.; Brandao, C. R. F. (2003). "Reproductive conflict in animal societies: hierarchy length increases with colony size in queenless ponerine ants". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 54: 71–79. doi:10.1007/s00265-003-0600-9. S2CID 11142025.
  39. ^ Monnin, T. (2006). "Chemical recognition of reproductive status in social insects". Annales Zoologici Fennici. 43: 515–530.
  40. ^ Tibbetts, E. A.; Dale, J. (2004). "A socially enforced signal of quality in paper wasp". Nature. 432 (7014): 218–222. Bibcode:2004Natur.432..218T. doi:10.1038/nature02949. PMID 15538369. S2CID 4428070.
  41. ^ Peeters, C.; Liebig, J.; Hölldobler, B. (2000). "Sexual reproduction by both queens and workers in the ponerine ant Harpegnathos saltator". Insectes Sociaux. 47 (4): 325–332. doi:10.1007/pl00001724. S2CID 8728206.
  42. ^ Hoover, S. E. R.; Keeling, C. I.; Winston, M. L.; Slessor, K. N. (2003). "The effect of queen pheromones on worker honey bee ovary development". Naturwissenschaften. 90 (10): 477–480. Bibcode:2003NW.....90..477H. doi:10.1007/s00114-003-0462-z. PMID 14564409. S2CID 22875850.
  43. ^ Ratnieks, F. L. W.; Visscher, P. K. (1989). "Worker policing in the honeybee". Nature. 342 (6251): 796–797. Bibcode:1989Natur.342..796R. doi:10.1038/342796a0. S2CID 4366903.
  44. ^ Endler, A.; Liebig, J.; Schmitt, T.; et al. (2004). "Surface Hydrocarbons of queen eggs regulate worker reproduction in a social insect". PNAS. 101 (9): 2945–2950. Bibcode:2004PNAS..101.2945E. doi:10.1073/pnas.0308447101. PMC 365725. PMID 14993614.
  45. ^ Oliveim, PS; Hölldobler, B (1991). "Agonistic interactions and reproductive dominance in Pachycondyla obscuricornis (Hymenoptera, Formicidae)". Psyche: A Journal of Entomology. 98 (2–3): 215–226. doi:10.1155/1991/64635.
  46. ^ a b c d Roseler, P.F.; Roseler, I.; Strambi, A.; Augier, R. (1984). "Influence of insect hormones on the establishment of dominance hierarchies among foundresses of the paper wasp, Polistes gallicus". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 15 (2): 133–142. doi:10.1007/bf00299381. S2CID 30682118.
  47. ^ a b Tibbetts, E. A.; Izzo, A. S. (2009). "Endocrine mediated phenotypic plasticity: Condition-dependent effects on juvenile hormone on dominance and fertility of wasp queens". Hormones and Behavior. 56 (5): 527–531. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2009.09.003. PMID 19751736. S2CID 6907877.
  48. ^ Clarke, F. M.; Faulkes, C. G. (7 August 1998). "Hormonal and behavioural correlates of male dominance and reproductive status in captive colonies of the naked mole–rat, Heterocephalus glaber". Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences. 265 (1404). The Royal Society: 1391–1399. doi:10.1098/rspb.1998.0447. PMC 1689228. PMID 9721687.
  49. ^ a b Faulkes, C. G.; Abbott, D. H. (1993). "Evidence that primer pheromones do not cause social suppression of reproduction in male and female naked mole-rats". Journal of Reproduction and Fertility. 99 (1): 225–230. doi:10.1530/jrf.0.0990225. PMID 8283442.
  50. ^ For example, fishes such as Oreochromis mossambicus: Creel, S. (2005). "Dominance, aggression and glucocorticoid levels in social carnivores". Journal of Mammalogy. 86 (2): 255–264. doi:10.1644/bhe-002.1.
  51. ^ a b c Muller, M. N.; Wrangham, R.W. (2004). "Dominance, cortisol and stress in wild chimpanzees" (PDF). Journal of Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 55 (4): 332–340. doi:10.1007/s00265-003-0713-1. hdl:2027.42/46903. S2CID 206939737.
  52. ^ Genuth, S. M. (1993). "The endocrine system". In Berne, R. M.; Levy, M. N. (eds.). Physiology. Vol. 3. St. Louis: Mosby Year Book. pp. 813–1024.
  53. ^ Sapolsky, R. M. (1992). "Cortisol concentrations and the social significance of rank instability among wild baboons". Journal of Psychoneuroendocrinology. 17 (6): 701–709. doi:10.1016/0306-4530(92)90029-7. PMID 1287688. S2CID 23895155.
  54. ^ Wang, F.; Zhu, J.; Zhu, H.; Zhang, Q.; Lin, Z.; Hu, H. (2011-11-04). "Bidirectional Control of Social Hierarchy by Synaptic Efficacy in Medial Prefrontal Cortex". Science. 334 (6056): 693–697. Bibcode:2011Sci...334..693W. doi:10.1126/science.1209951. ISSN 0036-8075. PMID 21960531. S2CID 15173160.
  55. ^ Sallet, J.; Mars, R. B.; Noonan, M. P.; et al. (2011-11-04). "Social Network Size Affects Neural Circuits in Macaques". Science. 334 (6056): 697–700. Bibcode:2011Sci...334..697S. doi:10.1126/science.1210027. PMID 22053054. S2CID 206536017.
  56. ^ a b Wang F, Kessels HW, Hu H (2014). The mouse that roared: neural mechanisms of social hierarchy. OCLC 931061361.
  57. ^ Zhou, Tingting; Zhu, Hong; Fan, Zhengxiao; et al. (14 July 2017). "History of winning remodels thalamo-PFC circuit to reinforce social dominance". Science. 357 (6347): 162–168. Bibcode:2017Sci...357..162Z. doi:10.1126/science.aak9726. PMID 28706064.
  58. ^ Digby, L. I.; Kahlenberg, S. M. (2002). "Female dominance in blue-eyed black lemurs". Primates. 43 (3): 191–199. doi:10.1007/BF02629647. PMID 12145400. S2CID 19508316.
  59. ^ Sauther, Michelle L. (1993). "Resource Competition in Wild Populations of Ringtailed Lemurs (Lemur Catta): Implications for Female Dominance". Lemur Social Systems and Their Ecological Basis. Springer. pp. 135–152. doi:10.1007/978-1-4899-2412-4_10. ISBN 978-1-4899-2414-8.
  60. ^ Young, Andrew L.; Richard, Alison F.; Aiello, Leslie C. (1990). "Female Dominance and Maternal Investment in Strepsirhine Primates". The American Naturalist. 135 (4): 473–488. doi:10.1086/285057. S2CID 85004340.
  61. ^ Drea, Christine M. (April 2007). "Sex and seasonal differences in aggression and steroid secretion in Lemur catta: Are socially dominant females hormonally 'masculinized'?". Hormones and Behavior. 51 (4): 555–567. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2007.02.006. PMID 17382329. S2CID 25398027.
  62. ^ von Engelhard, Nikolaus; Kappeler, Peter M.; Heistermann, Michael (2000). "Androgen levels and female social dominance in Lemur catta". Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences. 267 (1452): 1533–1539. doi:10.1098/rspb.2000.1175. PMC 1690709. PMID 11007329.
  63. ^ Drea, Christine M. (August 2009). "Endocrine Mediators of Masculinization in Female Mammals". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 18 (4): 221–226. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01640.x. S2CID 145424854.
  64. ^ a b Grebe, Nicholas M.; Fitzpatrick, Courtney; Sharrock, Katherine; Starling, Anne; Drea, Christine M. (September 2019). "Organizational and activational androgens, lemur social play, and the ontogeny of female dominance". Hormones and Behavior. 115: 104554. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2019.07.002. PMID 31276664. S2CID 195812654.
  65. ^ Wittig, Roman M.; Boesch, Christophe (1 August 2003). "Food Competition and Linear Dominance Hierarchy Among Female Chimpanzees of the Taï National Park". International Journal of Primatology. 24 (4): 847–867. doi:10.1023/A:1024632923180. S2CID 39695783.
  66. ^ Drea, C. M.; Weldele, M. L.; Forger, N. G.; et al. (1 May 1998). "Androgens and masculinization of genitalia in the spotted hyaena (Crocuta crocuta). 2. Effects of prenatal anti-androgens". Reproduction. 113 (1): 117–127. doi:10.1530/jrf.0.1130117. PMID 9713384.
  67. ^ Davies, Charli S.; Smyth, Kendra N.; Greene, Lydia K.; et al. (December 2016). "Exceptional endocrine profiles characterise the meerkat: sex, status, and reproductive patterns". Scientific Reports. 6 (1): 35492. Bibcode:2016NatSR...635492D. doi:10.1038/srep35492. PMC 5067592. PMID 27752129.
  68. ^ Perrin, P. G. (1955). "'Pecking order' 1927–54". American Speech. 30 (4): 265–268. doi:10.2307/453561. JSTOR 453561.
  69. ^ Schjelderup-Ebbe, T. (1975). "Contributions to the social psychology of the domestic chicken [Schleidt M., Schleidt, W. M., translators]". In Schein, M. W. (ed.). Social Hierarchy and Dominance. Benchmark Papers in Animal Behavior. Vol. 3. Stroudsburg, Pennsylvania: Dowden, Hutchinson and Ross. pp. 35–49. (Reprinted from Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 1922, 88:225-252.)
  70. ^ Rajecki, D. W. (1988). "Formation of leap orders in pairs of male domestic chickens". Aggressive Behavior. 14 (6): 425–436. doi:10.1002/1098-2337(1988)14:6<425::AID-AB2480140604>3.0.CO;2-#. S2CID 141664966.
  71. ^ Craig, J. V. (11 May 1978). Aggressive behavior of chickens: Some effects of social and physical environments (PDF). 27th Annual National Breeder's Roundtable. Kansas City. Archived from the original (PDF) on 16 February 2016. Retrieved 2013-08-24.
  72. ^ Drummond, Hugh; Edda Gonzalez; Jose Luis Osorno (1986). "Parent-Offspring Cooperation in the Blue-footed Booby (Sula nebouxii): Social Roles in Infanticidal Brood Reduction". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 19 (5): 365–372. doi:10.1007/bf00295710. S2CID 36417383.
  73. ^ Clarke, F. M.; Faulkes, C. G. (1997). "Dominance and queen succession in captive colonies of the eusocial naked mole-rat, Heterocephalus glaber". Proc Biol Sci. 264 (1384): 993–1000. Bibcode:1997RSPSB.264..993C. doi:10.1098/rspb.1997.0137. PMC 1688532. PMID 9263466.
  74. ^ West-Eberhard, M. J. (1969). "The social biology of polistine wasps". Mis. Publ. Zool. Univ. Michigan. 140: 1–101.
  75. ^ Strassmann & Meyer (1983). "Gerontocracy in the social wasp, Polistes exclamans". Animal Behaviour. 31 (1): 431–438. doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(83)80063-3. S2CID 54398769.
  76. ^ González, J. A.; Nascimento, F. S.; Gayubo, S. F. (2002). "Observations on the Winter Aggregates of Two Polistine Paper Wasps (Hymenoptera Vespidae Polistinae)". Tropical Zoology. 15 (1): 1–4. Bibcode:2002TrZoo..15....1G. doi:10.1080/03946975.2002.10531162.
  77. ^ Nogueira-Ferreira, F. H.; Silva-Matos, E. V.; Zucchi, R. (2009). "Interaction and Behavior of Virgin and Physogastric Queens in Three Meliponini Species (Hymenoptera, Apidae)". Genetics and Molecular Research. 8 (2): 703–708. doi:10.4238/vol8-2kerr008. PMID 19554769.
  78. ^ Klass, Keren; Cords, Marina (17 September 2015). "Agonism and dominance in female blue monkeys". American Journal of Primatology. 77 (12). Wiley: 1299–1315. doi:10.1002/ajp.22481. PMID 26378396. S2CID 8725428.
  79. ^ de Waal, Frans B. M.; Aureli, Filippo (1999). "Conflict Resolution and Distress Alleviation in Monkeys and Apes". In Carter, Carol Sue; Lederhendler, I. Izja; Kirkpatrick, Brian (eds.). The Integrative Neurobiology of Affiliation. MIT Press. pp. 119–140. ISBN 978-0-26253-158-0.
  80. ^ Hooper, Paul L.; Kaplan, Hillard S.; Jaeggi, Adrian V. (1 March 2021). "Gains to cooperation drive the evolution of egalitarianism". Nature Human Behaviour. 5 (7): 847–856. doi:10.1038/s41562-021-01059-y. PMID 33649461. S2CID 232089497.
  81. ^ Strier, Karen (2006). Primate Behavioural Ecology. Pearson Education. p. 5.
  82. ^ Alcock, John (2018). Animal Behavior: An Evolutionary Approach. Sinauer Associates. pp. 476–511.
  83. ^ Matsumura, Shuichi (1999). "The evolution of "egalitarian" and "despotic" social systems among macaques". Primates. 40 (1): 23–31. doi:10.1007/BF02557699. PMID 23179529. S2CID 23652944.
  84. ^ Watts, Heather E.; Holekamp, Kay E. (2007-08-21). "Hyena societies". Current Biology. 17 (16): R657–R660. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2007.06.002. PMID 17714659. S2CID 7723322.
  85. ^ Hewitt, Stacey E.; Macdonald, David W.; Dugdale, Hannah L. (2009). "Context-dependent linear dominance hierarchies in social groups of European badgers, Meles meles". Animal Behaviour. 77 (1): 161–169. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.09.022. S2CID 53205113.
  86. ^ Houpt, K. A.; Law, K.; Martinisi, V. (1978). "Dominance hierarchies in domestic horses". Applied Animal Ethology. 4 (3): 273–283. doi:10.1016/0304-3762(78)90117-7.
  87. ^ Clutton-Brock, J. (1987). A Natural History of Domesticated Mammals. Cambridge University Press. pp. 73–74.
  88. ^ Wroblewski, E. E.; Murray, C. M.; Keele, B. F.; Schumacher-Stankey, J. C.; Hahn, B. H.; Pusey, A. E. (2009). "Male dominance rank and reproductive success in chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii". Animal Behaviour. 77 (4): 873–885. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.12.014. PMC 2689943. PMID 19498952.
  89. ^ T. Nishida; et al. (January 1992). "Meat-sharing as a coalition strategy by an alpha male chimpanzee". ResearchGate.
  90. ^ "Humans Would be Better off if They Monkeyed Around Like the Muriquis".
  91. ^ Champagne, Melissa (November 2008). "Sperm Competition in the Florida Manatee (Trichechus manatus latirostris)". Hcnso Student Capstones.
  92. ^ "Elephants are socially complex".
  93. ^ Schreier, Amy L.; Swedell, Larissa (2009). "The fourth level of social structure in a multi-level society: Ecological and social functions of clans in hamadryas baboons". American Journal of Primatology. 71 (11): 948–955. doi:10.1002/ajp.20736. PMID 19670312. S2CID 205329272 – via ResearchGate.
  94. ^ Crisp, Rachel J.; Brent, Lauren J. N.; Carter, Gerald G. (2021). "Social dominance and cooperation in female vampire bats". Royal Society Open Science. 8 (7): 210266. Bibcode:2021RSOS....810266C. doi:10.1098/rsos.210266. PMC 8261227. PMID 34295524.
  95. ^ Furuichi, T. (2011). "Female contributions to the peaceful nature of bonobo society". Evolutionary Anthropology. 20 (4): 131–142. doi:10.1002/evan.20308. PMID 22038769. S2CID 17830996.

Further reading